Logan: Principles of Learning PRINCIPLE: OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING -- IMITATION Conditioning is facilitated if the organism has first observed another experienced organism subjected to the conditioning situation. Although observation learning is most commonly studied in operant/instrumental conditioning or discrimination learning, it is important to introduce the topic early in a survey of the basic Principles of Learning and it does indeed apply to classical conditioning. Which is more, observational learning is operationally a classical (sensory) conditioning procedure. That is to say, it involves the sequential presentation of stimulus events to the observer without regard to the observer's behavior. The principle states that the observing organism learns about the contingencies between the stimuli from observing a model being subjected to those contingencies. However, although such learning can be very significant, it is not as effective as actually being directly subjected to the contingencies. For example, a dog might be placed in the experimental room somewhat away from the experimental subject who is brought into the room, placed in the restraining apparatus, and exposed to the conditioning sequence of a metronome preceding food delivery to the model. Following this preexposure, the observer is a more willing participant in the experiment and displays more rapid conditioning. In fact, some CR may be observed on the very first trial. It should be noted that the observer must also be motivated at the time of observing the model, and there are some indications that there may be an optimal amount of preexposure for maximal benefit from the experience. Of special importance is the perceived similarity of the model to the observer; a dog profits more from watching another dog engaged in the conditioning situation. Observational learning would appear at first glance strongly to favor the cognitive interpretation of conditioning. After all, the dog has simply watched the experienced subject being put through its paces and yet behavior is modified by such observations. The behavioristic theorist, however, refers to this phenomenon as VICARIOUS LEARNING and contends that the observing subject actually makes incipient, fractional responses during the observation period. Specifically the observer certainly heard the metronome and clearly saw the food being delivered. The observer presumably also salivates at the sight of food, since that event has certainly been previously conditioned to salivation, and so the procedure is actually a special case of higher-order conditioning. Consistent with this general view is the fact that observing organisms often show emotional responses when another organism is exposed to an emotionally-significant event such as a painful shock. If one is correct that the organism vicariously responds to the US that is presented to another organism, then the evidence of observational learning does not resolve the basic controversy. The resolution of the cognitive-behavioristic controversy need not detain us from the empirical phenomenon of observational/vicarious learning. It is a well documented principle not only with humans but with many other mammals as well. As indicated, the principle also obtains in the other learning paradigms that we shall be discussing and it is important to note that imitation is learnable. Human children in particular are reinforced for imitative behavior while, for example, learning to talk. Hence, although observational/vicarious learning is not dependent on special training, its importance is very significantly increased in organisms who have indeed been taught to profit from the experience of others. One of the most familiar contexts for observational learning in modern society is television. At issue is the extent to which violence (and sex, etc.) on T.V. begets violence in the streets. What empirical evidence there is on the matter is consistent with the implications of the Principle of Observational Learning. Indeed, television has the potential of having a major impact on learning. Earlier it was noted that our attitudes, beliefs, and prejudices reflect classical conditioning of emotional responses. Many of these are learned vicariously. You may never personally have been maligned by a person of another race but observing others reveal these prejudices will strongly influence your opinion through the principle of observational learning. There is also the other side of the coin: many people think that "it can't happen to me." For example, many is the recovering alcoholic wh has convinced himself or herself that controlled social drinking is again possible. Much more often than not, this conviction turns out to be very much in error. TERMS: Conditioning (classical, higher-order, instrumental, operant, sensory), conditioned response, imitation, vicarious learning, discrimination learning, motivation.