Logan: Basic Principles of Learning PRINCIPLE: PRIMARY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT If a response is followed by the termination of an innately aversive event, there will result an increase in the probability that that response will recur on future exposures to that aversive event. This principle is frequently referred to as ESCAPE LEARNING. An organism exposed to a naturally aversive event emits many and varied responses. For example, a rat exposed to a mildly painful electric shock through the grid floor of an experimental apparatus will squeal, prance around, jump, and probably urinate and defecate. If in the course of these activities, the rat happens to jump across a barrier into an adjacent safe compartment, that response has received negative reinforcement and hence is more likely to occur the next time the shock occurs in the apparatus. (Note that negative reinforcement is not the same as punishment, which we will consider separately. Reinforcement always increases response probability and, in this case is called negative only because it is occasioned by a decrease in stimulation.) As in this example, the aversive event is usually turned off by the escape response but negative reinforcement can also be produced by a decrease in the aversiveness of the event. Such an incomplete reduction is not as effective as total termination. Acquisition of a response on the basis of negative reinforcement is typically quite rapid, depending on the intensity of the aversive event. Generally speaking, escape learning is faster the more noxious the event that is involved although there is a complication to this rule. Escape learning is difficult when the escape response is incompatible with the innate responses elicited by the aversive event, and these responses may depend on the intensity of the stimulus. It is more difficult for a rat to learn to turn off a strong shock by standing still than if the shock is weak. Escape learning is a model of learning reinforced by drive reduction. It is first assumed that a naturally aversive event arouses a primary pain drive in the organism, which instigates a high level of activity. If one of the responses that occurs in this process removes the aversive event, the drive is correspondingly reduced and this outcome reinforces (strengthens) that response. It is a principle of biological survival; drive energizes the organism into action and whatever response removes the aversive state is reinforced and hence learned. Some theorists have used comparable reasoning with respect to the other primary drives. For example, we can conceptualize hunger as being an aversive state and food is reinforcing because it reduces that state. As one also encounters in the context of primary positive reinforcement, there is no way of knowing in advance what events are aversive to an organism. Indeed, escape learning is typically used as a procedure for finding out whether an event is aversive. That is to say, we know that an event is aversive to the organism if it will learn a response to turn it off but some caution is required in using this circular reasoning. For example, it is not very surprising that a rat will learn a response to turn off very bright lights, but a rat will also learn a response to turn on dim lights in a dark environment. There are other contexts in which the optimal level of stimulation is not at zero. The room temperature of the environment is a case in point because organisms learn escape responses when it is either too hot or too cold. The most dramatic example of too-little stimulation has been obtained from humans confined in a soundproof room where almost nothing happens. Except for periodically being given food, the subject has nothing to do except lay down and sleep or pace around the room. After several days in such an environment, the subject will readily expend effort on any response that will provide some kind of visual or auditory stimulation and will before long, exercise the escape option of quitting the experiment. At least to some extent, uncomfortable situations are simply unavoidable in everyday life and we learn to cope with them as best we can. Often, we can readily beat a hasty retreat and there is little need for learning, but many are the homespun remedies for maladies ranging from a headache to the common cold. A more challenging task is to disengage oneself diplomatically from an unpleasant interpersonal situation such as the married couple who have discovered some unbearable trait in their spouses. Finally, the academic drop-out is frequently a person for whom school, at whatever level including college, has become unpleasant so that leaving the field appears to be the best escape. TERMS: Escape learning, reinforcement (primary, negative), escape response.