Logan: Basic Principles of Learning PRINCIPLE: OPPONENT MOTIVATIONAL PROCESSES The termination of an emotionally-significant event is followed by an opposite emotional state; with repeated stimulation, there is a decrease in the original emotional state and an increase in the strength and duration of the opponent state. Our emotional lives, our "ups" and "downs," do not come and go abruptly from some middle-of-the-road neutral level. Instead, the time course of our reactions to an emotionally-significant event (pleasant or unpleasant) follows a rather complex pattern characterized by (1) a peak of emotional reaction, (2) a lower steady level for the duration of the stimulus, (3) a reversal to the opposite emotional state after termination of the stimulus and finally (4) a gradual return to the normal level. The the higher the emotional reaction to the event, the higher the opposing process after the stimulus has terminated. The pattern is something like a pendulum that, after being displaced in one direction, swings back into the other direction. An early observation suggestive of this pattern was made by Pavlov in the context of differential conditioning. He observed that, if S+ is presented soon after S-, the dog is likely to show an excessively strong conditioned response. He termed this phenomenon POSITIVE INDUCTION because his interpretation was that S- produced a strong inhibitory state but when it ended, there was a temporary super-excitatory state which would increase responding to any positive stimulus. Conversely, NEGATIVE INDUCTION arises when an S- is presented shortly after an S+. In this case, the idea is that S+ leads to a highly excitatory state which, after S+ ends, passes through a temporary inhibitory state that will further depress the tendency to respond to S-. This temporary reversal is characteristic of many of our biological systems. For example, a negative after-image (dark spots) after looking at a bright light is a familiar experience. You may have enjoyed the childhood activity of spinning around in one direction, and then standing still while having the sensation of spinning in the other direction. In contrast to these sensory experiences, the opponent motivational processes have a unique and very important property: Frequent exposure decreases the primary process and increases both the intensity and duration of the opponent process. The nature of the effect can most readily be appreciated by consideration of drug-taking behavior, say marijuana. For the first few exposures, the experience while smoking pot is a "rush" or heightened state of euphoria. The opponent process after the joint is finished, is one of desire for more, which fairly rapidly recovers to the normal resting state. The veteran user, however, the addict, experiences only a moderate state of contentment during the smoking episode, which is followed by an agony of withdrawal and a prolonged period of craving. It must be emphasized that the primary process can as well be emotionally negative, resulting in a positive opponent motivational state. For example, during the first efforts at jogging, the experience is one of moderate pain and discomfort, followed by a pleasant state of relaxation. With repeated experiences, however, the primary reaction when jogging is less negative, may almost be exciting, but it is followed by a prolonged happy feeling of exhilaration. In sum, we can indeed become trapped into engaging in behaviors that are basically aversive because it makes us feel so good to stop. Functionally, opponent motivational processes provide a form of positive feedback. Feedback is said to be positive when it naturally leads to further performance of the behavior producing the feedback, which is contrasted with negative feedback that tends to terminate the behavior. Consider the behavior of eating. Normally, the primary feedback effect is to a decreased desire for food. However, the opponent motivational process adds a source of positive feedback because, one you begin to eat, each time you start to stop eating there is an aroused craving to continue eating. In effect, you may soon be eating just to be eating, a fact that you can readily convince yourself of by deliberately stopping eating sooner than usual and observing your own feelings. For a very few minutes, you will continue to want to eat, but once the opponent motivational process has run its course, you will find that you really aren't hungry. The inclusion of the Principle of Opponent Motivational Processes may be somewhat premature. In contrast to the other principles described in this book, which have enjoyed at least a quarter-century, and typically a half-century of refining experimental analysis, the present principle is a relative newcomer to the scene. I chose to include it, however, to emphasize that the principles are not completely static or "written in stone". Our perception of them is continually being revised, and new ones are continually vying for recognition. And in this case, the potential practical significance of the principle justifies early presentation. One of the distressing statistics of modern society is the frequency of suicide, especially among college students. The facts are that few of these young people really want to die, but they have been overwhelmed by some apparent devastating emotional experience. The Principle of Opponent Motivational Processes implies the familiar saying that every dark cloud has a silver lining. Anything you can do to help yourself or a despondent friend weather the depths or despair will be repaid handsomely when the pendulum swings. TERMS: Opponent motivation process, feedback (negative, positive).