Logan: Basic Principles of Learning PRINCIPLE: STIMULUS GENERALIZATION A response whose probability of occurrence has been increased by any conditioning procedure will also tend to occur in the presence of stimuli similar to that which prevailed at the time of conditioning. The Principle of Stimulus Generalization has been observed in every conditioning context. For example, during conditioning, a tone could precede food delivery in the classical paradigm, could signal the availability of food reinforcement for emitting an operant response, or could indicate whether running down a runway would result in food reward. Subsequently, the extent to which the tone stimulus has gained selective control over behavior is best measured by changing that stimulus to determine whether there is a stimulus generalization decrement. If the response is weaker as a result of a stimulus change, then clearly the stimulus was exerting some degree of control over the response. There are many factors that affect the amount of generalization. Generally speaking, generalization is greater after irregular conditions of reinforcement, be that partial reinforcement or a variation in the amount, delay, or quality of the reinforcing event. Occasional punishment of a rewarded response also increases generalization. The implication of the Principle of Stimulus Generalization is a gradient showing the amount of generalized response tendency as a function of the similarity of a test stimulus to the training stimulus. Although the empirical Principle of Stimulus Generalization is generally accepted and widely employed in various contexts, there is no compelling theoretical account of the relevant phenomena. It would be simple enough to say that two tones sound somewhat alike, or two visual displays look somewhat alike, but it is not at all apparent why irregular conditions of reinforcement should result in greater generalization. Specifically, it would have to be assumed that partial reinforcement of one tone makes a second tone somehow sound more like the conditioned tone, but this is hardly intuitively reasonable. Quite probably the account will ultimately relate to the fact that the factors that influence generalization also influence persistence in the same way. Generalized response tendencies have relatively little persistence; extinction with a generalized stimulus proceeds much more rapidly than to the original stimulus. Even more important than reinforcement variability in increasing generalization and persistence is variation in the stimulus situation during learning. If a response has been conditioned to a wide variety of stimuli, it will naturally generalize broadly to an even wider range of stimulus situations. (Note that reinforcement variability may be seen as a special case of stimulus variability, and hence persistence may be viewed as a special case of generalization to conditions that occur during extinction.) There are several special cases of stimulus generalization, one of which is called CROSS-MODAL generalization. This refers to a tendency to respond to a stimulus presented in one sensory modality after learning occurred to a stimulus in another modality. For example, a person might learn the morse code of dots and dashes seen as a series of flashing lights, and then be able to translate the code heard as a series of clicks. An especially important case of cross-modal generalization in human behavior is called DYNAMIC TRANSMISSION. This refers to the tendency to generalize between objects or events and the words used to refer to them. It is this fact that makes verbal instructions possible; if you are given directions to turn left at the next traffic light, you must generalize from the spoken word "traffic light" to the actual sight of the light. As with most principles of learning, dynamic transmission is not always adaptive. Some people cringe at the mere mention of the word "snake," and many people are upset by the use of four-letter words that are only sounds in the air or marks on a page. Which is worse, attaching a label such as "slow," "homely," or "stutterer" may cause the very outcome being described. Stimulus generalization is most apparent in everyday life when it can be described as either overgeneralization or undergeneralization. A student who concludes that all mathematics courses are terrifying as a result of some early difficulties with arithmetic has persistent overgeneralized reactions. A student who does not see the basic Principles of Learning operating in day-to-day activities reflects an undergeneralization from the classroom to the outside world. The principle invades even our own self-image; the "born-loser" syndrome is an overgeneralization based on a few unfortunate experiences that may, as a matter of fact, turn into a self-fulfilling prophecy. TERMS: Stimulus control, stimulus generalization decrement, generalization (cross modal, stimulus), generalization gradient, stimulus similarity.