Paths to Softlifting Intention

Laurie L. R. Schatzberg (rattner@unm.edu)
John D. Schatzberg
Richard A. Reid
University of New Mexico
Albuquerque, NM, 87131-1221

Introduction

Softlifting is an individual act of copying software without the legal right to do so, and is distinct from two other forms of software piracy: commercial piracy (for distribution and sale) and corporate piracy (institutional lack of appropriate means to control software distribution within an organization) [Hornik, 1994]. That the concept of software piracy has been distinguished into three classes of behaviors reflects both the growing concern and understanding of the nature of these activities. The earliest attempts to control software piracy were aimed at minimizing the vendors' losses from both commercial and corporate piracy, which began with the introduction of microcomputers in the 1980s. The formation of the US Software Publishers Association (SPA) in 1988, and, subsequently, the international Business Software Alliance, resulted in a highly visible spate of successful lawsuits against US and international firms engaging in either commercial or corporate piracy (BSA, 1994). The software industry's focus on commercial and corporate piracy reflects a reasonable effort to protect themselves from tens of billions of lost sales dollars worldwide, since these types of pirates are both easier to detect and easier entities from which to recover lost revenues.

Our work focuses on individual level of piracy, whereby individuals copy software for personal use. Softlifting research draws upon not only the piracy literature, but also on the broader domains of social psychology (theories of planned behavior and of cognitive dissonance), ethics, and information systems. This empirical work complements broader works on ethical theory and concepts advanced most recently by Walsham (1996), who relates classical ethical theory to the practice of IS; Conger et al. (1995), who emphasizes the need to understand users so that policies, rewards and punishments can be developed; Laudon (1995), who contributes a typology of ethical theories within which the IS community can reason about ethical issues; and, most broadly, Mason (1995), who calls upon the IS community to help shape the direction of society.

This study builds directly on the growing literature attempting to identify demographic characteristics that can predict the likelihood of softlifting. Most recently, Simpson et al. (1994) hypothesize a two-stage model in which five factors (stimulus, socio-cultural, legal, personal, and situational) affect one's ethical decision process - and that the decision process alone accounts for one's softlifting behaviors. They conclude that gender, religion, personal gain and situational factors affect the decision to softlift, and that ethical perceptions of softlifting have no bearing upon the practice. Logsdon et al. (1994) report prevalence of softlifting among university students with little support for their hypothesis that softlifting is related to level of moral judgment, suggesting that this issue has low moral intensity. These recent results are largely consistent with earlier works in the field: Im and Van Epps (1992), who found soft-lifting prevalent in accredited business schools; Solomon and O'Brien (1990), and later, O'Brien et al. (1991), who found that softlifting is both wide-spread and deemed acceptable among university students; and Taylor and Shim (1993), who report that while business executives softlift less than academicians, both view softlifting as mildly unethical.

Model and Hypotheses

Our main dependent variable is the Intention to softlift (see Fig. 1), and we hypothesize that Intention is a function of two main constructs: Background and Attitudes. Background factors are assumed stable over time for an individual and directly influence the Intention to softlift. We also hypothesize that Background influences Attitudes, a construct that varies over time. Attitudes about softlifting separately influence the Intention to softlift. (This influence will either amplify or diminish the Background influence, depending on the individual's stage of resolving any cognitive dissonance.) Each of the constructs is comprised of several factors. While the full model is the subject of an ongoing study, this paper focuses exclusively upon "Path 1," the influence of Background upon Intention to softlift.

We investigate these hypotheses defining Path 1: (1) perceptions of higher rates of softlifting among peers yields greater softlifting Intention; (2) a strong ethical profile is associated with weaker Intention to softlift; (3) increased levels of computer use at home yield higher levels of softlifting; (4) increasing levels of purchased software yields lower levels of softlifting; (5) increased knowledge of the piracy statutes are associated with lower levels of softlifting; and (6) gender has no effect on Intention to softlift. We include gender in our set of independent variables because its role in the existing literature is not well understood. A complete discussion of these hypotheses is available from the authors.

Methodology

Three hundred seventy-three students from several MBA, Executive MBA, and undergraduate business and engineering classes were given a questionnaire covering demographics, access to computers, knowledge of copyright law, attitudes about copying software, as well as their willingness to do so under several scenarios. The questionnaire includes twenty items from Paulhus (1990) Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR) which has been shown to detect and measure impression management bias. A total of ninety-seven responses were discarded: 85 for incompleteness and 12 for impression management bias, leaving 276 usable responses (74% usable response rate). We re-used the BIDR items in the study to develop a set of ethics factors within our Background construct, because these items have strong face validity as indicators of an individual's practical ethical profile. To our knowledge, this is the first study to use these items for broader purposes.

The dependent variable, Intention to softlift, is investigated using three sets of scenarios concerning respondents' intent to copy software for friends' entertainment, school, and work purposes. If respondents agree to softlift for any item within the entertainment scenario, they are classified as demonstrating Intention to softlift. Respondents who answer "no" for all of the entertainment scenarios are classified as demonstrating no Intention to softlift. The same scheme is used to determine softlifting Intentions for school and work scenarios.

Factor analysis is employed to identify the orthogonal components of the twenty BIDR items in conjunction with three questions relating to the peer environment. This analysis indicates seven (7) significant dimensions (eigenvalues greater than 1). The corresponding factor scores are used as the independent variables Peer Views and Ethical Factor 1 through Ethical Factor 6 within a regression framework. As previously discussed, we employ several additional independent variables: computer Use-at-home, measured in sessions per week; Purchase level (lowest level in the intercept, with Purchase level2 through 6 in the model); Knowledge of laws (number of correct responses out of 10 questions); and Gender (dummy variable with 0=female; 1=male).

Findings

Our results support the proposed model in that the Background construct is shown to be a significant predictor of Intention to softlift. There is variation, however, among the predictors for the three different softlifting scenarios. Table 1 presents our regression findings with significant variables shown in bold. We hypothesized that softlifting would be more prevalent among people who frequently use a computer at home, a hypothesis that is partly supported by our model when the intended software is for school purposes (but not for entertainment or work). Perhaps computer use at home is irrelevant with respect to softlifting work-related software due to the specialized nature of the software, or a perception that work-related software should be purchased by one's employer. The meaning of our findings with respect to entertainment is not clear.

The hypothesis that people who purchase greater amounts of software are less likely to softlift is supported by the estimates shown for Purchase level 6: an indication that there is a significant difference between the lowest and highest purchase levels. This result suggests that not only do people who purchase a considerable amount of software tend to follow rules, but that they also expect their friends to do so. This interpretation also suggests that they internalize a deontological ethical perspective that suggests individuals are guided by a set of unvarying principles (Walsham, 1996).

Peer Views is a significant predictor of softlifting in all three scenarios, and is consistent with other works (Peace and Galletta, 1996). In addition, at least one Ethical Factor is significant in each scenario, with four of the six significant for work purposes. While we did not expect this variation, the result is broadly consistent with consequential ethical theories (Walsham, 1996) that individuals consider the context of behaviors in judging their ethicality.

These Ethical Factors have not yet been investigated to understand fully how they relate to constructs from social psychological or ethical theories. Our results indicate that respondents' Intention to softlift is influenced by ethical considerations, a conclusion that differs from previous research. Since variables chosen to represent ethics constructs differ across studies, it is possible that the works are identifying different sub-factors included within a broad ethics construct. Our results suggest the opportunity to explore more fully ways of operationalizing the ethics construct.

Conclusion

Softlifting is as complex a phenomenon as it is prevalent, and individuals are continually faced with the opportunity to copy software without the authority to do so. Demonstrating the Intention to softlift under a variety of scenarios has been shown to be a function of several variables in one's Background. This result supports the existence of Path 1 on our proposed model, and work is underway to investigate the two remaining paths. To the extent that we can uncover the correlates to softlifting, the IS profession will be positioned to offer guidance on methods and policies to minimize the occurrence. Further work is required to understand more fully the dynamics leading to softlifting.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Professors Robert D. Rogers and Gautam Vora for their insightful assistance with the data analysis. Any remaining errors are the full responsibility of the authors. Appreciation to the UNM Anderson Schools of Management Research Allocations Committee for funding in support of this work.

References

Available from the first author

TABLE 1: REGRESSION RESULTS

Items in each factor available from the authors

INTENTION

TO SOFTLIFT

FOR ENT'MENT
INTENTION TO SOFTLIFT FOR SCHOOL
INTENTION TO SOFTLIFT FOR WORK
Parameter

estimate


T
Parameter

estimate


T
Parameter

estimate


T
Peer Views 0.068388 3.182* 0.079734 4.172* 0.057448 2.102*
Ethical factor1 0.066586 3.150* 0.065237 3.471* 0.123989 4.614*
Ethical factor2 0.011797 0.5670.018746 1.012 0.0249870.944
Ethical factor3 0.028897 1.2810.019439 0.969 0.070105 2.444*
Ethical factor4 0.044438 2.049* 0.027044 1.4020.065824 2.388*
Ethical factor5 0.02267 1.0820.017202 0.923 0.07703 2.891*
Ethical factor6 -0.006924 -0.333 -0.004069 -0.220.026704 1.009
Use-at-home 0.005111 1.5620.006181 2.124* 0.001474 0.354
Purchase level2 -0.001688 -0.033 0.0546641.194 0.011318 0.173
Purchase level3 -0.045487 -0.665 -0.065853 -1.082-0.061815 -0.711
Purchase level4 -0.002043 -0.021 -0.025935 -0.3-0.010117 -0.082
Purchase level5 0.012422 0.120.051157 0.561 0.0214420.164
Purchase level6

(highest)

-0.237792 -2.47* -0.218574 -2.554* -0.31482 -2.573*
Knowledge of laws -0.014322 -1.503 -0.011488 -1.356-0.018839 -1.555
Gender 0.064466 1.4270.007866 0.196 0.0620981.081
F Value
Pr>F
F Value
Pr>F
F Value
Pr>F
2.834 0.00043.895 0.00014.589 0.0001
R2
0.1405
R2
0.1835
R2
0.2093
Adj R2
0.0909
Adj R2
0.1364
Adj R2
0.1637

*Significant at the .01 level